The Handbook of Speech Perception

Здесь есть возможность читать онлайн «The Handbook of Speech Perception» — ознакомительный отрывок электронной книги совершенно бесплатно, а после прочтения отрывка купить полную версию. В некоторых случаях можно слушать аудио, скачать через торрент в формате fb2 и присутствует краткое содержание. Жанр: unrecognised, на английском языке. Описание произведения, (предисловие) а так же отзывы посетителей доступны на портале библиотеки ЛибКат.

The Handbook of Speech Perception: краткое содержание, описание и аннотация

Предлагаем к чтению аннотацию, описание, краткое содержание или предисловие (зависит от того, что написал сам автор книги «The Handbook of Speech Perception»). Если вы не нашли необходимую информацию о книге — напишите в комментариях, мы постараемся отыскать её.

A wide-ranging and authoritative volume exploring contemporary perceptual research on speech, updated with new original essays by leading researchers Speech perception is a dynamic area of study that encompasses a wide variety of disciplines, including cognitive neuroscience, phonetics, linguistics, physiology and biophysics, auditory and speech science, and experimental psychology.
, Second Edition, is a comprehensive and up-to-date survey of technical and theoretical developments in perceptual research on human speech. Offering a variety of perspectives on the perception of spoken language, this volume provides original essays by leading researchers on the major issues and most recent findings in the field. Each chapter provides an informed and critical survey, including a summary of current research and debate, clear examples and research findings, and discussion of anticipated advances and potential research directions. The timely second edition of this valuable resource:
Discusses a uniquely broad range of both foundational and emerging issues in the field Surveys the major areas of the field of human speech perception Features newly commissioned essays on the relation between speech perception and reading, features in speech perception and lexical access, perceptual identification of individual talkers, and perceptual learning of accented speech Includes essential revisions of many chapters original to the first edition Offers critical introductions to recent research literature and leading field developments Encourages the development of multidisciplinary research on speech perception Provides readers with clear understanding of the aims, methods, challenges, and prospects for advances in the field
, Second Edition, is ideal for both specialists and non-specialists throughout the research community looking for a comprehensive view of the latest technical and theoretical accomplishments in the field.

The Handbook of Speech Perception — читать онлайн ознакомительный отрывок

Ниже представлен текст книги, разбитый по страницам. Система сохранения места последней прочитанной страницы, позволяет с удобством читать онлайн бесплатно книгу «The Handbook of Speech Perception», без необходимости каждый раз заново искать на чём Вы остановились. Поставьте закладку, и сможете в любой момент перейти на страницу, на которой закончили чтение.

Тёмная тема
Сбросить

Интервал:

Закладка:

Сделать

The DIVA model proposes that early vocal development involves a closed‐loop imitation process driven initially by two stages of babbling and later a vocal learning stage that directly involves corrective feedback processing. In the first babbling phase, random articulatory motions generate somatosensory and acoustic consequences, and a mapping of the developing vocal tract as a speech device takes place. Separately, infants learn the perceptual categories of their native language. This is crucial to the model. As Guenther (1995, p. 599) states, “the model starts out with the ability to perceive all of the sounds that it will eventually learn to produce.” When the second stage of babbling begins, it involves the mapping between phonetic categories developed through perceptual learning and articulation. The babbling during this period tunes the feedback system to permit corrective responses to detected errors. In the next imitation phase, infants hear adult syllables that they try to reproduce. In a cyclical process involving sensory feedback from actual productions and better feedforward commands, the system shapes the early utterances toward the native language.

Simulations by Guenther and his students support the logic of this account. However, there are significant concerns. First among these is that the data supporting this process are weak or, in the case of MacDonald et al.’s (2012) results, contradict the hypothesis. Early speech feedback processing and the shaping of speech production targets is not well attested. Second, the proposal relies on a strong relationship between the representations of speech perception and speech production. Surprisingly, this relationship is controversial.

Models like DIVA predict a phenomenon that is frequently assumed but is not strongly supported by actual data – babbling drift. The hypothesis that the sounds of babbling drift over time was first proposed by Roger Brown (1958). Brown suggested that the phonetic repertoire in the babbling of infants slowly begins to resemble the phonetics of the language environment that they are exposed to and begins to not include sounds that are absent from the native language. As the review by Best et al. (2016) indicates, the support for this idea is mixed, particularly from transcription studies and perceptual studies where naive listeners attempted to identify the language environment of the infant’s babbling.

There is broad agreement that early babbling has common characteristics across languages and a somewhat limited phonetic repertoire. The evidence from later babbling lacks this broad consensus. Older transcription studies of late babbling were often plagued by small sample sizes and bias issues inherent in transcription. Studies with larger sample sizes, however, still show conflicting patterns of results. For example, de Boysson‐Bardies and Vihman (1991) reported that the prevalence of consonants of different manners and places of articulation in the babbling of 12‐month old infants from English, French, Japanese, and Swedish homes corresponded to the distributions of consonants in their language environments. In contrast, a number of other transcription studies have failed to find such differences (e.g. Kern, Davis, & Zink, 2009; Lee, Davis, & MacNeilage, 2010).

Figure 42 Average F1 circles and F2 triangles frequencies estimates across - фото 16

Figure 4.2 Average F1 (circles) and F2 (triangles) frequencies estimates across time for adults (top panel), young children (middle panel), and toddlers (bottom panel). The formant frequencies have been normalized to the average baseline frequencies. The shaded area indicates when subjects were given altered auditory feedback (from MacDonald et al., 2012).

Source: MacDonald et al., © 2012, Elsevier.

Another approach has been to use recordings of infants babbling as perceptual stimuli and ask adult listeners to categorize what native language the infants have. These studies have also shown mixed results, with some experiments reporting that listeners can discriminate the home language of the infants (e.g. de Boysson‐Bardies, Sagart, & Durand, 1984) while others showed no perceptual difference (e.g. Thevenin et al., 1985). The more serious concern about these studies is that listeners were likely tuning into prosodic differences in the babbling rather than the segmental differences that would be predicted by babbling drift. The ability to perceptually distinguish the language of babbling has been shown for low‐pass filtered stimuli (e.g. Whalen, Levitt, & Wang, 1991) and this supports the idea that it is prosodic differences that are driving these results. A recent controlled study (Lee et al., 2017) with a large number of stimuli found that perceptual categorizations of Chinese‐ and English‐learning babies’ utterances at 8, 10, and 12 months of age were only reliable for a small subset of the stimuli (words or canonical syllables that resembled words). These effects were modest and suggest that early lexical development rather than babbling may be where the home language shows its earliest influence.

Direct measurements of babbling acoustics have shown evidence for babbling drift, albeit only small effects. For example, Whalen, Levitt, and Goldstein (2007) measured voice onset time (VOT) in French‐ and English‐learning infants at ages 9 and 12 months. There were no differences in VOT or in the duration of prevoicing that was observed. However, there was a greater incidence of prevoicing in the French babies which corresponds to adult French–English differences.

The most serious concern from the existing data is that there is no evidence for speech‐production tuning of targets based on production errors. MacDonald et al. (2012) data suggest that young children do not correct errors. However, there are several caveats to that conclusion. First, the magnitude of the perturbation may have to be within a critical range and the perturbations for all ages in MacDonald et al. were the same in hertz. It is possible that younger children require larger perturbations to elicit compensations. A related issue is that the perturbations may have been within the noisy categories that the children were producing. The variability of production may be an indicator of the category status. However, even if this were true, it begs the question: How could an organism learn to produce adult targets under these conditions? The challenges are enormous. Juveniles in all species have vocal tracts that do not match their parents’ vocal tracts. Birds and other species show marked production variance as juveniles (e.g. Bertram et al., 2014). There is no obvious feedback base mechanism that permits the mapping from adult targets to young productions (see Messum & Howard, 2015). Error correction as normally envisioned in motor control may not be engaged.

Perception–production interaction

This puzzle reflects the general problem of understanding the relationship between the processes of listening to speech and producing it. Liberman (1996, p. 247) stated:

In all communication, sender and receiver must be bound by a common understanding about what counts; what counts for the sender must count for the receiver, else communication does not occur. Moreover, the processes of production and perception must somehow be linked; their representation must, at some point, be the same.

This is certainly true in a very general sense but the roles played in communication by the auditory signal that reaches the listener and by the signal that reaches the speaker are dramatically different. For the listener, the signal is involved in categorical discrimination and information transmission, while for the talker the signal is primarily thought to influence motor precision and error correction. These two issues are not independent but are far from equivalent. The problem for researchers is that the perception and production of speech are so intrinsically intertwined in communication that it is difficult to distinguish the influence of these “two solitudes” of speech research on spoken language.

Читать дальше
Тёмная тема
Сбросить

Интервал:

Закладка:

Сделать

Похожие книги на «The Handbook of Speech Perception»

Представляем Вашему вниманию похожие книги на «The Handbook of Speech Perception» списком для выбора. Мы отобрали схожую по названию и смыслу литературу в надежде предоставить читателям больше вариантов отыскать новые, интересные, ещё непрочитанные произведения.


Отзывы о книге «The Handbook of Speech Perception»

Обсуждение, отзывы о книге «The Handbook of Speech Perception» и просто собственные мнения читателей. Оставьте ваши комментарии, напишите, что Вы думаете о произведении, его смысле или главных героях. Укажите что конкретно понравилось, а что нет, и почему Вы так считаете.

x