Daniel Gardner - The Science of Fear
Здесь есть возможность читать онлайн «Daniel Gardner - The Science of Fear» весь текст электронной книги совершенно бесплатно (целиком полную версию без сокращений). В некоторых случаях можно слушать аудио, скачать через торрент в формате fb2 и присутствует краткое содержание. ISBN: , Издательство: Penguin Group (USA) Incorporated, Жанр: Психология, Политика, Прочая научная литература, на английском языке. Описание произведения, (предисловие) а так же отзывы посетителей доступны на портале библиотеки ЛибКат.
- Название:The Science of Fear
- Автор:
- Издательство:Penguin Group (USA) Incorporated
- Жанр:
- Год:неизвестен
- ISBN:9780525950622
- Рейтинг книги:3 / 5. Голосов: 1
-
Избранное:Добавить в избранное
- Отзывы:
-
Ваша оценка:
- 60
- 1
- 2
- 3
- 4
- 5
The Science of Fear: краткое содержание, описание и аннотация
Предлагаем к чтению аннотацию, описание, краткое содержание или предисловие (зависит от того, что написал сам автор книги «The Science of Fear»). Если вы не нашли необходимую информацию о книге — напишите в комментариях, мы постараемся отыскать её.
The Science of Fear — читать онлайн бесплатно полную книгу (весь текст) целиком
Ниже представлен текст книги, разбитый по страницам. Система сохранения места последней прочитанной страницы, позволяет с удобством читать онлайн бесплатно книгу «The Science of Fear», без необходимости каждый раз заново искать на чём Вы остановились. Поставьте закладку, и сможете в любой момент перейти на страницу, на которой закончили чтение.
Интервал:
Закладка:
As expected, the students who got the easy-to-imagine symptoms and who imagined themselves contracting the disease rated the risk highest. Next came the two groups who did not do the imagining exercise. The lowest risk estimate came from those who got the hard-to-imagine symptoms and did the imagining exercise. This proved something important about imagining: It’s not merely the act of imagining that raises Gut’s estimate of how likely something is, it’s how easy it is to imagine that thing. If imagining is easy, Gut’s estimate goes up. But if it is a struggle to imagine, it will feel less likely for that reason alone.
It may be a little surprising to think that the act of imagining can influence our thoughts, but in many different settings—from therapy to professional sports—imagining is used as a practical tool whose effectiveness is just as real as the famous placebo effect. Imagination is powerful. When the ads of lottery corporations and casinos invite us to imagine winning—one lottery’s slogan is “Just Imagine”—they do more than invite us to daydream. They ask us to do something that elevates our intuitive sense of how likely we are to win the jackpot—which is a very good way to convince us to gamble. There is no “just” in imagining.
This isn’t the only potential problem with Gut’s use of the Example Rule. There’s also the issue of memory’s reliability.
Most people think memory is like a camera that captures images and stores them for future retrieval. Sure, sometimes the camera misses a shot. And sometimes it’s hard to find an old photo. But otherwise, memory is a shoebox full of photos that directly and reliably reflect reality.
Unfortunately, this isn’t even close to true. Memory is better described as an organic process. Memories routinely fade, vanish, or transform— sometimes dramatically. Even the strongest memories—those formed when our attention is riveted and emotions are pumping—are subject to change. A common experiment memory researchers conduct is tied to major news, such as the September 11 terrorist attacks. In the days immediately following these spectacular events, students are asked to write how they heard about it: where they were, what they were doing, the source of the news, and so on. Years later, the same students are asked to repeat the exercise and the two answers are compared. They routinely fail to match. Often the changes are small, but sometimes the entire setting and the people involved are entirely different. When the students are shown their original descriptions and are told that their memories have changed, they often insist their current memory is accurate and the earlier account is flawed—another example of our tendency to go with what the unconscious mind tells us, even when doing so is blatantly unreasonable.
The mind can even fabricate memories. On several occasions, Ronald Reagan recalled wartime experiences that were later traced to Hollywood movies. These were apparently honest mistakes. Reagan’s memory simply took certain images from films he had seen and converted them into personal memories. Reagan’s mistake was caught because, as president, his comments were subjected to intense scrutiny, but this sort of invention is far more common than we realize. In one series of experiments, researchers invented scenarios such as being lost in a shopping mall or staying overnight in a hospital with an ear infection. They then asked volunteers to imagine the event for a few days or to write down how they imagine it played out. Then, days later, the researchers interviewed the subjects and discovered that between 20 and 40 percent believed the imagined scenarios had actually happened.
A more basic problem with the Example Rule is that it is biased, thanks to the way our memories work. Recent, emotional, vivid, or novel events are all more likely to be remembered than others. In most cases, that’s fine because it’s precisely those sorts of events that we actually need to remember.
But the bias in our memory will be reflected in Gut’s judgments using the Example Rule—which explains the paradox of people buying earthquake insurance when the odds of an earthquake are lowest and dropping it as the risk rises. If an earthquake recently shook my city, that memory will be fresh, vivid, and frightening. Gut will shout: Be afraid! Buy insurance! But if I’ve been living in this place for decades and there has never been an earthquake, Gut will only shrug. Not even scientists issuing warnings will rouse Gut because it doesn’t know anything about science. It knows only what the Example Rule says, and the Example Rule says don’t worry about earthquakes if you have to struggle to remember one happening.
“Men on flood plains appear to be very much prisoners of their experience, ” researcher Robert Kates wrote in 1962, bemoaning the fact that people erect buildings despite being told that a flood must inevitably come. We saw the same dynamic at work following the terrible tsunami that swept across the Indian Ocean on December 26, 2004. Afterward, we learned that experts had complained about the lack of a warning system. It didn’t cost much, the experts had argued, and a tsunami was bound to come. It was a pretty esoteric subject, however, and no one was interested. Many people had never even heard the word tsunami until the day 230,000 lives were taken by one. And when that happened, the whole world started talking about tsunamis. Why was there no warning system in place? Could it happen here? Is our warning system good enough? It was the hot topic for a month or two. But time passed and there were no more tsunamis. Memories faded and so did the concern. For now, at least. A team of scientists has warned that one of the Canary Islands off the coast of Africa is fractured and a big chunk of the island will someday crash into the ocean—causing a mammoth tsunami to race across the Atlantic and ravage the coast from Brazil to Canada. Other scientists dispute these findings, but we can safely assume that, should this occur, interest in this esoteric subject would revive rather abruptly.
Experience is a valuable thing and Gut is right to base intuitions on it, but experience and intuition aren’t enough. “Experience keeps a dear school,” Benjamin Franklin wrote, “but fools will learn in no other.”
Franklin wrote those words in the mid-eighteenth century. From the perspective of a human living in the early twenty-first century, that’s a very long time ago, but in evolutionary terms it might as well have been this morning. The brain inside Franklin’s head was particularly brilliant, but it was still, in its essentials, no different than yours or mine or that of the person who first put seeds in the ground 12,000 years ago—or that of the human who first daubed some paint on a cave wall 40,000 years ago.
As we have seen, the world inhabited by humans changed very little over most of that sweep of time. And then it changed almost beyond description. The first city, Ur, was founded only 4,600 years ago and never got bigger than 65,000 people. Today, half of all humans live in cities—more than 80 percent in some developed countries.
Even more sweeping than the transformation of the physical environment is the change in how we communicate. The first crude writing—with symbols scratched into soft clay—appeared about 5,000 years ago. Gutenberg invented modern printing a mere five and a half centuries ago, and it was at this stage that Ben Franklin published his witticism about the limits of experience.
The first photograph was taken 180 years ago. Radio appeared a century ago, television thirty years later. It was only forty-eight years ago that the first satellite message was relayed—a Christmas greeting from U.S. President Eisenhower.
Then came cable television, fax, VCR, e-mail, cell phones, home video, digital, twenty-four-hour cable news, and satellite radio. Less than twenty years ago, the rare journalist who knew of the Internet’s existence and wrote about it would put quotation marks around the word and carefully explain the nature of this unfathomable contraption. Today, it is embedded in the daily lives of hundreds of millions of people and occasionally touches the lives of billions more. Google, iPod, Wikipedia, YouTube, Facebook, MySpace: All these words represent globe-spanning information channels with immense and unfolding potential to change societies. And yet, as I write this sentence, only one—Google—has even existed for ten years.
Читать дальшеИнтервал:
Закладка:
Похожие книги на «The Science of Fear»
Представляем Вашему вниманию похожие книги на «The Science of Fear» списком для выбора. Мы отобрали схожую по названию и смыслу литературу в надежде предоставить читателям больше вариантов отыскать новые, интересные, ещё непрочитанные произведения.
Обсуждение, отзывы о книге «The Science of Fear» и просто собственные мнения читателей. Оставьте ваши комментарии, напишите, что Вы думаете о произведении, его смысле или главных героях. Укажите что конкретно понравилось, а что нет, и почему Вы так считаете.