Andrew Radford - Linguistics An Introduction [Second Edition]

Здесь есть возможность читать онлайн «Andrew Radford - Linguistics An Introduction [Second Edition]» весь текст электронной книги совершенно бесплатно (целиком полную версию без сокращений). В некоторых случаях можно слушать аудио, скачать через торрент в формате fb2 и присутствует краткое содержание. Год выпуска: 2008, Издательство: Cambridge, Жанр: Языкознание, на английском языке. Описание произведения, (предисловие) а так же отзывы посетителей доступны на портале библиотеки ЛибКат.

Linguistics An Introduction [Second Edition]: краткое содержание, описание и аннотация

Предлагаем к чтению аннотацию, описание, краткое содержание или предисловие (зависит от того, что написал сам автор книги «Linguistics An Introduction [Second Edition]»). Если вы не нашли необходимую информацию о книге — напишите в комментариях, мы постараемся отыскать её.

This textbook is a self-contained introduction to linguistics for beginning students. It offers a unified approach to language from several perspectives. A language is a complex structure represented in the minds of its speakers, and this book introduces the tools necessary for understanding this structure. In addition, it focuses on how small children acquire their native language; the psychological processes which are involved in mature speakers producing and understanding language; linguistic difficulties which arise as a consequence of brain damage or genetic disorders; and additional issues which arise when we consider individual speakers as part of a social community.Written by a team based at one of the world's leading centres for linguistic teaching and research, the second edition of this highly successful textbook offers a unified approach to language, viewed from a range of perspectives essential for students' understanding of the subject. Using clear explanations throughout, the book is divided into three main sections: sounds, words, and sentences. In each, the foundational concepts are introduced, along with their application to the fields of child language acquisition, psycholinguistics, language disorders, and sociolinguistics, giving the book a unique yet simple structure that helps students to engage with the subject more easily than other textbooks on the market. This edition includes a completely new section on sentence use, including an introduction and discussion of core areas of pragmatics and conversational analysis; coverage of sociolinguistic topics, introducing communities of practice; a wealth of new exercise material and updated further reading.

Linguistics An Introduction [Second Edition] — читать онлайн бесплатно полную книгу (весь текст) целиком

Ниже представлен текст книги, разбитый по страницам. Система сохранения места последней прочитанной страницы, позволяет с удобством читать онлайн бесплатно книгу «Linguistics An Introduction [Second Edition]», без необходимости каждый раз заново искать на чём Вы остановились. Поставьте закладку, и сможете в любой момент перейти на страницу, на которой закончили чтение.

Тёмная тема
Сбросить

Интервал:

Закладка:

Сделать

one?’). In terms of the analysis outlined here, this means that the two sentences have the respective structures in (463) below (with want being a finite form in

463a and a non-finite infinitive form in 463b): (463) a.

[

φ

φ

CP [C ] [TP I

[T +FIN] [VP [V want ] one]]]

b.

[

φ

φ

CP [C ] [TP me [T –FIN] [VP [V want] one]]]

On this view, children alternate between using finite verbs and infinitives in contexts where adults use finite verbs: for this reason, Ken Wexler and his

co-researchers have dubbed the relevant stage the Optional Infinitive stage

(sometimes abbreviated to OI stage). This stage typically lasts until around the child’s fourth birthday (with the use of non-finite clauses in finite contexts gradually becoming less and less frequent as the child gets older).

A related phenomenon which we find during the relevant stage is that children alternate between using tensed and untensed verb forms in contexts where adults require tensed verbs (i.e. verbs inflected for present/past tense). This pattern is illustrated by the sentences in (464) below (produced by Claire at ages two years to two years, one month):

(464) a.

David did it. Claire did it. Bear did it. Claire fell down. Claire woke up.

Happened the hammer? Look I found. That goes little one. That one goes

another one. There goes another one. Goes there. Goes here.

b.

Pixie eat dinner. Jane help dinner time. Bunny stand up. Cow fall down. Claire close it. Claire do puzzle. Jane do it. That go there. Chair go there. That one fit.

Daddy sit in chair. Raggedy Ann sit down. Raggedy Ann lie down. Porcupine

lie down. Raggedy Ann stay there. Raggedy Ann to wake up. Jane see

Mommy. Pig say oink (reply to ‘What does the pig say?’)

The sentences in (464a) are finite clauses containing a finite verb like happened/

goes overtly inflected for tense, but those in (464b) appear to be non-finite clauses containing an untensed verb like eat/go. In terms of the framework we are using here, Claire fell down and Claire fall down will have the respective simplified structures (465a, b):

358

senten ces

(465) a.

[

φ

φ

CP [C ] [TP Claire [T +FIN] [VP [V fell ] down]]]

b.

[

φ

φ

CP [C ] [TP Claire [T –FIN] [VP [V fall] down]]]

Both clauses are CP+TP+VP structures, but they differ in that T is finite in (465a) and so the verb fell is overtly inflected for past tense, whereas T is non-finite in

(465b) and so the verb remains in the uninflected form fall (i.e. the same form as we find in infinitives). Once again, we see the familiar pattern of children alternating between finite and non-finite forms in finite contexts.

Children’s nominals

Up to this point, we have concentrated on the clause structures pro-

duced by young children, noting that they sometimes produce non-finite clauses in finite contexts, and so, for example, omit auxiliaries (or finite verb inflections like present/past tense -s/-d) where adults require them. We find a similar pattern of development in relation to children’s nominal structures. From around two years of age, children start to produce adult-like DP structures of the form determiner +

noun, using both definite determiners like the/this/that and indefinites such as a/

another/some. However, alongside determinate nominals containing overt defi-

nite or indefinite determiners, we also find children producing bare nominals which contain a noun but no determiner (in contexts where adults would require a determiner), as illustrated by the following sentences produced by Claire at ages two years to two years, one month:

(466) a.

There’s the hat. Piggie see the water. Baby drink the coffee. Daddy sitting in the chair. Horsie swimming in the pool. Do the green one. Put that mommy in

the carriage.

b.

It’s a baby. It’s a dolly. It’s a girl. There’s a spider. There’s a bunny. There’s another one. There goes another one. Put another fence.

c.

Daddy sit in chair. Girl sleeping. Baby eating dinner. Baby eating juice. Claire do puzzle. Pig say oink. Read book. Ring bell. See flower.

The italicised nominals in (466a) are DPs headed by the definite determiners the/that, and likewise those in (466b) are DPs headed by the indefinite determiners a/another. Since Claire is clearly able to form DPs at this stage, it seems reasonable to assume that all her nominals are DPs (as indeed must be the case if

principles of UG specify that all nominals are D-projections). But this in turn means that bare nominals such as those italicised in (466c) must also be DPs; and since they contain no overt determiner, they must be headed by a null determiner.

Note that Claire doesn’t just use null determiners in contexts where adults do (e.g. with proper names like Claire), but also in contexts where adults require an overt determiner (e.g. modifying a singular count noun like chair/girl/puzzle, etc.). In other words, just as she omits auxiliaries in obligatory contexts, so too she omits determiners in obligatory contexts (i.e. in contexts where adults would require an overt determiner).

Children’s sentences

359

Research conducted by Teun Hoekstra and Nina Hyams has suggested that there

are systematic parallels between the role of T in clauses and the role of D in nominals. They note that just as the tense specification of T serves to anchor a clause in time, so too the definiteness specification of D serves to anchor a nominal in space. On this basis, they argue that definiteness and tense are two different manifestations of a single common property, which they refer to as finiteness. In the terminology of Hoekstra and Hyams, nominals which contain an overt determiner are finite, whereas those which lack an overt determiner in a context where adults would require one are non-finite. This means that a sentence such as Pig say oink (which Claire used in reply to ‘What does the pig say?’) will have the structure (467) (simplified by not showing the internal structure of the VP say oink): (467)

CP

TP

DP

T'

C D

N T

VP

ϕ

ϕ –FIN

pig

ϕ –FIN

say oink

In the same way as the overall clause is non-finite by virtue of containing a TP

headed by a non-finite T, so too the subject DP is non-finite by virtue of being headed by a non-finite D.

Hoekstra and Hyams argue that children’s finite clauses always have finite

subjects (so that they say The doggy is barking but not *Doggy is barking), but that their non-finite clauses can have either finite or non-finite subjects (so that they say both The doggy barking and Doggy barking). They maintain that this

follows from UG principles – more specifically from the specifier-head agreement relation which holds (universally) between a finite T and its subject. Since there is no agreement relation between a non-finite T and its subject, there are no finiteness restrictions on the choice of subject in a non-finite clause.

Hoekstra and Hyams’s claim that children sometimes use a non-finite D

in contexts where adults use a finite D can be extended in interesting ways

to account for the fact that young children often alternate between producing genitive and accusative possessors – as the following examples produced by a three-year-old boy called Nicholas illustrate (the data kindly being provided by Joseph Galasso):

(468) a.

I want my key

b.

I want me duck

(469) a.

What’s his name?

b.

What’s him name?

(470) a.

Where’s Zoe’s bottle?

b.

Where Daddy car?

360

senten ces

In the (a) examples in (468–70), the italicised possessor my/his/Zoe’s has genitive case, whereas in the (b) examples the possessor me/him/Daddy is accusative. Why should young children alternate between genitive and accusative possessors?

Читать дальше
Тёмная тема
Сбросить

Интервал:

Закладка:

Сделать

Похожие книги на «Linguistics An Introduction [Second Edition]»

Представляем Вашему вниманию похожие книги на «Linguistics An Introduction [Second Edition]» списком для выбора. Мы отобрали схожую по названию и смыслу литературу в надежде предоставить читателям больше вариантов отыскать новые, интересные, ещё непрочитанные произведения.


Отзывы о книге «Linguistics An Introduction [Second Edition]»

Обсуждение, отзывы о книге «Linguistics An Introduction [Second Edition]» и просто собственные мнения читателей. Оставьте ваши комментарии, напишите, что Вы думаете о произведении, его смысле или главных героях. Укажите что конкретно понравилось, а что нет, и почему Вы так считаете.

x